Question
Question 1]After reading the Atlantic article below, please answer taking into consideration your: take challenges your experience and maybe making some important contributions . 2]
Question
1]After reading the Atlantic article below, please answer taking into consideration your:
- take
- challenges
- your experience
- and maybe making some important contributions .
2] Answer revealing compromises and what is itthat you can already visualize that will be the more challenging parts of data analytics
The Atlantic article
The Myth of 'I'm Bad at Math'.Basic ability in the subject isn't the product of good genes, but hard work.
ByMiles Kimball,Noah Smith,andQuartz
"I'm just not a math person."
We hear it all the time. And we've had enough. Because we believe that the idea of "math people" is the most self-destructive idea in America today. The truth is, you probablyarea math person, and by thinking otherwise, you are possibly hamstringing your own career. Worse, you may be helping to perpetuate a pernicious myth that is harming underprivileged childrenthe myth of inborn genetic math ability.
Is math ability genetic? Sure,to some degree. Terence Tao, UCLA's famous virtuoso mathematician, publishes dozens of papers in top journals every year, and is sought out by researchers around the world to help with the hardest parts of their theories. Essentially none of us could ever be as good at math as Terence Tao, no matter how hard we tried or how well we were taught. But here's the thing: We don't have to! Forhigh-schoolmath, inborn talent is much less important than hard work, preparation, and self-confidence.
How do we know this? First of all, both of us have taught math for many yearsas professors, teaching assistants, and private tutors. Again and again, we have seen the following pattern repeat itself:
Different kids with different levels of preparation come into a math class. Some of these kids have parents who have drilled them on math from a young age, while others never had that kind of parental input.
On the first few tests, the well-prepared kids get perfect scores, while the unprepared kids get only what they could figure out by winging itmaybe 80 or 85%, a solid B.
The unprepared kids, not realizing that the top scorers were well-prepared, assume that genetic ability was what determined the performance differences. Deciding that they "just aren't math people," they don't try hard in future classes, and fall further behind.
The well-prepared kids, not realizing that the B students were simply unprepared, assume that they are "math people," and work hard in the future, cementing their advantage.
Thus, people's belief that math ability can't change becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
The idea that math ability is mostly genetic is one dark facet of a larger fallacy that intelligence is mostly genetic. Academic psychology journals are well stocked with papers studying the world view that lies behind the kind of self-fulfilling prophecy we just described. For example,Purdue University psychologist Patricia Linehan writes:
A body of research on conceptions of ability has shown two orientations toward ability. Students with an Incremental orientation believe ability (intelligence) to be malleable, a quality that increases with effort. Students with an Entity orientation believe ability to be nonmalleable, a fixed quality of self that does not increase with effort.
The "entity orientation" that says "You are smart or not,end of story," leads to bad outcomesa result that has been confirmed bymany other studies. (The relevance for math is shown by researchers at Oklahoma Citywho recently foundthat belief in inborn math ability may be responsible for much of the gender gap in mathematics.)
Psychologists Lisa Blackwell, Kali Trzesniewski, and Carol Dweckpresented these alternatives to determine people's beliefs about intelligence:
You have a certain amount of intelligence, and you really can't do much to change it.
You can always greatly change how intelligent you are.
They found that students who agreed that "You can always greatly change how intelligent you are" got higher grades. But as Richard Nisbett recounts in his bookIntelligence and How to Get It,they did something even more remarkable:
Dweck and her colleagues then tried to convince a group of poor minority junior high school students that intelligence is highly malleable and can be developed by hard work...that learning changes the brain by forming new...connections and that students are in charge of this change process.
The results? Convincing students that they could make themselves smarter by hard work led them to work harder and get higher grades. The intervention had the biggest effect for students who started out believing intelligence was genetic. (A control group, who were taught how memory works, showed no such gains.)
But improving grades was not the most dramatic effect, "Dweck reported that some of her tough junior high school boys were reduced to tears by the news that their intelligence was substantially under their control." It is no picnic going through life believing you were born dumband are doomed to stay that way.
For almost everyone,believing that you were born dumband are doomed to stay that wayis believing a lie.IQ itself can improve with hard work. Because the truth may be hard to believe, here is a set of links about some excellent books to convince you that most people can become smart in many ways, if they work hard enough:
The Art of Learningby Josh Waitzkin
Moonwalking with Einsteinby Joshua Foer
The Talent Codeby Daniel Coyle
Talent is Overratedby Geoff Colvin
So why do we focus on math? For one thing, math skills are increasingly important for getting good jobs these daysso believing you can't learn math is especially self-destructive. But we also believe that math is the area where America's "fallacy of inborn ability" is the most entrenched. Math is the great mental bogeyman of an unconfident America. If we can convince you that anyone can learn math, it should be a short step to convincing you that you can learn just about anything, if you work hard enough.
Is America more susceptible than other nations to the dangerous idea of genetic math ability? Here our evidence is only anecdotal, but we suspect that this is the case. While American fourth and eighth gradersscore quite wellin international math comparisonsbeating countries like Germany, the UK and Swedenour high-schoolers underperform those countriesby a wide margin. This suggests that Americans' native ability is just as good as anyone's, but that we fail to capitalize on that ability through hard work. In response to the lackluster high school math performance, some influential voices in American education policy have suggested simplyteaching less mathfor example,Andrew Hacker has calledfor algebra to no longer be a requirement. The subtext, of course, is that large numbers of American kids are simply not born with the ability to solve forx.
We believe that this approach is disastrous and wrong. First of all, it leaves many Americans ill-prepared to compete in a global marketplace with hard-working foreigners. But even more importantly, it may contribute to inequality. A great deal of research has shown that technical skills in areas like software are increasingly making the difference between America's upper middle class and its working class. While we don't think education is a cure-all for inequality, we definitely believe that in an increasingly automated workplace, Americans who give up on math are selling themselves short.
Too many Americans go through life terrified of equations and mathematical symbols. We think what many of them are afraid of is "proving" themselves to be genetically inferior by failing to instantly comprehend the equations (when, of course, in reality, even a math professor would have to read closely). So they recoil from anything that looks like math, protesting: "I'm not a math person." And so they exclude themselves from quite a fewlucrative career opportunities. We believe that this has to stop. Our view is shared by economist and writer Allison Schrager, who has written two wonderful columns inQuartz(hereandhere), that echo many of our views.
One way to help Americans excel at math is to copy the approach of the Japanese, Chinese, and Koreans. InIntelligence and How to Get It,Nisbett describes how the educational systems of East Asian countries focus more on hard work than on inborn talent:
"Children in Japan go to school about 240 days a year, whereas children in the United States go to school about 180 days a year."
"Japanese high school students of the 1980s studied 3 hours a day, and that number is likely to be, if anything, higher today."
"[The inhabitants of Japan and Korea] do not need to read this book to find out that intelligence and intellectual accomplishment are highly malleable. Confucius set that matter straight twenty-five hundred years ago."
"When they do badly at something, [Japanese, Koreans, etc.] respond by working harder at it."
"Persistence in the face of failure is very much part of the Asian tradition of self-improvement. And [people in those countries] are accustomed to criticism in the service of self-improvement in situations where Westerners avoid it or resent it."
We certainly don't want America's education system to copyeverythingJapan does (and we remain agnostic regarding the wisdom of Confucius). But it seems to us that an emphasis on hard work is a hallmark not just of modern East Asia, but of America's past as well. In returning to an emphasis on effort, America would be returning to its roots, not just copying from successful foreigners.
Besides cribbing a few tricks from the Japanese, we also have at least one American-style idea for making kids smarter:treat people who work hard at learning as heroes and role models. We already venerate sports heroes who make up for lack of talent through persistence and grit; why should our educational culture be any different?
Math education, we believe, is just the most glaring area of a slow and worrying shift. We see our country moving away from a culture of hard work toward a culture of belief in genetic determinism. In the debate between "nature vs. nurture," a critical third elementpersonal perseverance and effortseems to have been sidelined. We want to bring it back, and we think that math is the best place to start.
Step by Step Solution
There are 3 Steps involved in it
Step: 1
Get Instant Access to Expert-Tailored Solutions
See step-by-step solutions with expert insights and AI powered tools for academic success
Step: 2
Step: 3
Ace Your Homework with AI
Get the answers you need in no time with our AI-driven, step-by-step assistance
Get Started